Participles, Adjectives, and the Role of Argument Structure
نویسندگان
چکیده
The focus of this paper is a construction which is surprisingly rarely scrutinised: the impersonal passive of the intransitive. Although it sometimes receives a brief mention in discussions of the passive, and although the impersonal passive label is often wrongly given to morpholexical impersonal constructions, there are as yet no thorough analyses of the impersonal passive available for any language. In this paper, I offer an analysis of this construction in Polish, where it is made up of a tensed auxiliary or copula, and a participle commonly referred to as the passive participle. 1 The impersonal passive of the intransitive – an introduction A example of an impersonal passive of the intransitive in Polish is given in (1), with the assumption that no neuter singular referent can be found in the context of this sentence which could be interpreted as the antecedent of its ‘dropped’ subject. The agent in impersonal passives, downgraded to an oblique, is optional and frequently left unexpressed. The -n-/-tparticiple has the SG.N ending -e used in situations when agreement breaks down, such as here, where there is no subject for the participle to agree with: (1) By!o codziennie sprz"tane (przez firm#). was.3SG.N daily clean.PART.SG.N (by company) ‘There has been cleaning every day (by a company).’ Sentence (1) is an example of a predicative use of the -n-/-tparticiple in an impersonal construction, i.e. a construction without a subject. Its personal counterpart is the common personal passive, where the participle has to agree with its subject in gender and number; depending on the grammarical gender of the subject, the participle will have one of the following endings, in the singular: masculine -y, feminine -a, or neuter -e; and in the plural: masculine human -i, or other than masculine human -e. In Table 1 below, (1) is repeated in (3) and shown next to its personal counterpart in (2). Although the subject of the personal variant can bear any number or gender, in the table below it is illustrated with a noun of neuter gender and singular number (this minimises the number of variables for an easier comparison of the examples). In the typological literature, a participial personal passive is sometimes referred to as an ‘objective resultative’. This is in contrast with another type of construction, the so-called ‘possessive resultative’, in which the participle is a member of a secondary predicate which is part of the clausal object of a personal active verb ‘have’. The bottom row of Table 1 shows a relatively familiar personal ‘possessive resultative’ in (4), and a virtually unstudied impersonal ‘possessive resultative’ in (5). It is worth noting that Polish possessive resultatives have a more neutral possessive interpretation than their English translations. In the absence of an oblique agent, Polish possessive resultatives do not exclude the interpretation that the possessor may have been the agent.
منابع مشابه
The redundancy of lexical categories
In this paper I argue that the familiar lexical category labels, N, V, A, P or equivalently the features such as [±N, ±V] are redundant in a theory which admits a level of argument structure. I modify Zwart’s (1992) conception of a-structure by arguing that major class members always include a ‘referential role’: for nouns, (for ‘eventuality’) for verbs and (‘attribute’) for adjecti...
متن کاملModern Greek Deverbal Adjectives in -tos: a lexical semantic approach
With this study, a detailed semantic classiication of Modern Greek deverbal adjectives ending in-tos is attempted and the semantic properties of the-tos and-menos deverbal adjectives are contrasted. Traces of an inherited argument structure are detected in the case of-menos, but no such evidence is available for the adjectives in-tos. 1 The problem It has been claimed in the literature (cf. Set...
متن کاملHow Can We Define The “Participles” in Mongolic Languages: Two Problems In Shinekhen Buryat
This paper aims to examine the function of participles in Shinekhen Buryat, which is one of the Mongolic languages spoken in Manchuria, and suggests the following two points: first, we cannot define the prototypical participles in Shinekhen Buryat as inflectional verb to adjective transposition, since they contain certain features that differ from adjectives; second, we recommend a recategoriza...
متن کاملFrom Event Structure to Scale Structure: Degree Modification in Deverbal Adjectives
An issue of interest to work in areas ranging from lexical semantics to natural language processing (especially automatic text generation) to language pedagogy is the semantics of degree modification by expressions like very, well, and much. Focusing on the case of deverbal gradable adjectives (such as acquainted), we show that the selective behavior of different degree modifiers provides an im...
متن کاملPredication and movement in passive
This paper reconciles conflicting evidence on the nature of adjectival passives. Some evidence suggests that externalization of an internal argument in adjectival passives is a lexical operation not involving syntactic movement, while other evidence suggests that adjectival passive participles project an internal argument in the syntax. It is proposed that a subject may be base generated in a p...
متن کاملThe Effect of Dynamic Assessment of Toulmin Model through Teacher- and Collective-Scaffolding on Argument Structure and Argumentative Writing Achievement of Iranian EFL Learners
Considering the paramount importance of writing logical arguments for college students, this study investigated the effect of dynamic assessment (DA) of Toulmin model through teacher- and collective-scaffolding on argument structure and overall quality of argumentative essays of Iranian EFL university learners. In so doing, 45 male and female Iranian EFL learners taking part in the study were r...
متن کامل